Events occur over some time interval, that
is, events involve a change which occurs over time, a moving
object, a moving observer, or both. Movement of objects is distinguished
from movement of observers by differences in the optical transformations
which occur in the visual field. Movement of objects within the
environment cause local changes in the optic array, while observer
movement yields global optic flow, and thus the deformations
in the local and global optic flow provide the observer with
information about events.
The necessity of optic flow for the perception
of objects and events is evident in the observation that large
changes go unnoticed if the optic flow is disrupted. This can
be achieved experimentally by altering presentations during saccadic
eye movement, or more simply, by introducing a grey blanking
image between successive displays in which changes occur. (see
Simons & Levin, 1997 for a review).
An example of change blindness - did you notice the change?
See the change now?.
Another demonstration of the necessity, and
sufficiency of optic flow for the perception of objects and events
is the point light displays first presented by Johannsson (1973). Johannsson filmed actors wearing luminous
patches on joint centres under dark conditions. Presentation
of stationary images of the actors conveyed little information,
but rich perceptual information was obtained from even brief
presentations of movement. Observers were able to distinguish
between male and female walkers, determine the identify of walking
friends, and the mass of weights being lifted by the actors.
A simple simple point display
More recent experiments have utilised point-light
displays in complex sporting tasks to assess the role of expertise
in the access to relevant information which specifies events
such as the type of shot played, or about to be played, by an
opponent.
A more complex point light display.
Selective attention
Both natural and artificial environments provide
far more information that can be assimilated simultaneously.
Selective attention is the process by which the salient cues
are selected and combined to provide the information required
to support the control of action.
The ability to selectively attend to saliant
cues and obtain the information required develops with experience.
For example, expert squash players obtain information about the
subsequent flight of an approaching ball from the movement of
their opponent's arm, that is, well before the ball is struck,
while novices appear unable to access this information and must
rely of the movement of the racquet and ball (Abernethy, 1987).
Visual search patterns have been studied in
an attempt to understand expert/novices differences to obtain
information from complex displays. The search patterns are in
part determined by knowledge and prior experience. For example,
skilled radiologists employ search patterns which roughly correspond
to the probability distribution of the location of abnormalities
(Kundel, 1974). Other differences appear
to correspond to differences in skill. For example, novices drivers
make more fixations on the road edge and speedometer, while experienced
drivers spend more time looking further ahead (Mourant &
Rockwell, 1972).
|